REBLOGGED FROM THE SLATE By William Saletan
Earlier this year, I wrote two articles about BDSM—bondage,
dominance/submission, and sadomasochism. I argued that BDSM, unlike
homosexuality, was inherently problematic and wasn’t an orientation. Defenders
of BDSM—Dan Savage, Jessica Wakeman, Clarisse Thorn, Jillian Keenan, and dozens
of Slate commenters—wrote back, rejecting these arguments. Then, two months
ago, Dutch psychologists published a study of kinksters and mental health. I
started digging around. There isn’t much quantitative research on this
population, but I found a few decent studies that can help us clarify this
debate. Is BDSM sick? Let’s look at the evidence.
1.
How many people do BDSM? There’s only one good
random-sample survey on this question. It was taken in Australia a decade ago.
Nearly 20,000 people between the ages of 16 and 59 were interviewed by phone.
In the whole sample, 1.8 percent of men and 1.2 percent of women answered yes
to the question, “In the last 12 months, have you been involved in B&D or
S&M?” (The question went on to explain, “That’s bondage and discipline,
sadomasochism, or dominance and submission.”) Among respondents who were
sexually active, the BDSM minority barely increased, to 2.0 percent of men and
1.4 percent of women. Among those who had a sexual partner in the previous
year, the figure was 2.2 percent of men and 1.3 percent of women.
That’s roughly equivalent to the sexually active gay
population, as measured by similar self-reporting. In the Australian survey,
the authors reported, “less than 2 per cent of men and women” said they’d “had
sex with a same-sex partner in the past year.” The percentage of respondents
who said they’d ever had a gay sexual experience (not just in the last year)
was higher—six percent of men, nine percent of women—and presumably the same is
true of BDSM. In the Dutch study, for instance, 448 respondents accessed and
completed a BDSM survey through a Web site devoted to personal secrets. Of
these, three percent “indicated having had previous BDSM experience.”
2.
Is it an orientation? Previously, I argued that
homosexuality is fixed (an orientation) but that BDSM is flexible (a
lifestyle). Kinksters replied that BDSM, too, is an orientation. What do the
data show? Mostly flexibility. In a study of Finnish BDSM enthusiasts, 27
percent “endorsed a statement suggesting that only sadomasochistic sex could
satisfy them,” but only five percent “no longer practiced ordinary sex.”
Furthermore, 40 percent had changed their “preference” or “behavior” (in the
authors’ words) from sadism to masochism or vice versa. In another study,
conducted in southern California, “32% of the sample indicated that BDSM play
occurred less than half the time they spent in sexual activity with partners,
and just 11.2% indicated that BDSM play was their only form of sexual
activity.” The core group, dedicated to BDSM, seems vastly outnumbered by
dabblers.
3.
Is it physically dangerous? That depends on what you’re
doing. In the Finnish study, bondage and flagellation were standard: More than
80 percent of the sample had done them in the preceding 12 months. The riskier
stuff was far less common: piercing
(done by 21 percent of the sample), skin branding (17 percent), hypoxyphilia
(suffocation games, also known as breath play—17 percent), electric shocks (15
percent), and knives or razor blades (13 percent). The California study found a
similar pattern: Bondage, flogging, and spanking were standard (more than 80
percent had done them), but other practices—“fire play” (20 percent), “piercing
play” (20), cutting (14), branding (9), and scarification (5)—were rarer. Some
potentially dangerous activities were surprisingly common—“electric play” (42
percent), “knife scenes” (40), and “breath play” (27)—though in many cases, the
implements were probably just props. It looks as though about 20 percent of
these folks are actually cutting, burning, zapping, or partially suffocating
each other.
That’s a minority, but it’s still worrisome. In the Finnish
sample, those who said they’d previously suffered sexual abuse—23 percent of
the women, and 8 percent of the men—were particularly problematic. According to
the authors, “Visits to a physician because of injuries obtained during sm-sex
were significantly more common among the abused respondents (11.1%) than among
the non-abused respondents (1.8%).”
BDSM community leaders preach the importance of “safe
words”—prearranged signals that the restrained, flogged, or dominated
participant can use to withdraw consent and stop the action. In the Finnish
study, 90 percent of the sample said they “sometimes” incorporated such words
in their encounters. But fewer than half did so “without exception.” That, too,
is a problem.
4.
Is it mentally unhealthy? For the most part, no. The
surveys vary, so let’s take them one by one. The California study, conducted by
Pamela Connolly of theCalifornia Graduate Institute, found a “significantly
higher level of narcissism” in its BDSM sample than in the general population.
Connolly esimated that 30 percent of people in the sample were clinically
significant on that scale. Theoretically, a high narcissism score implied
“little interest in give-and take in social life,” but Connolly cautioned that
it could signify “personality strengths as well as personality pathology.” Only
two of the 132 participants met the criteria for pathological narcissism, and
Connolly noted an “absence of borderline pathology.”
Likewise, the sample “scored relatively highly on almost all
of the scales” related to dissociative symptoms, suggesting a “higher
prevalence of a dissociative syndrome.” Yet only one respondent met the
criteria for dissociative identity disorder. Compared with the general
population, the sample showed a “significantly higher level of histrionic
features,” which increased the risk of “fear of genuine autonomy,” “a need for
repeated signs of acceptance and approval,” and an “indiscriminate search for
stimulation and affection.” But Connolly added that this result might have been
skewed by geography, since the Los Angeles BDSM community favored and fostered
exhibitionism.
Connolly reported no significant differences on “most
measures of PTSD, on obsessive-compulsion or on depression or anxiety.” She
emphasized that “no evidence was found to support the notion that clinical
disorders—including depression, anxiety, and obsessive-compulsion—are more
prevalent” in the BDSM population.
A Canadian study published that same year (2006) also
defended BDSM. “Masochists were not found to be more prone to psychological
distress or mental instability than the other groups,” the authors wrote, and “we
found no support for the idea that masochists were more inclined to engage in
escapist behaviors such as drug-taking.” Furthermore, “the SM participants
scored equivalent to or lower than the non-SM comparison group on
Authoritarianism.” But the authors conceded that their small sample—93
people—made it statistically difficult to find differences.
The Dutch study, which drew hundreds of BDSM respondents
from an online forum, found that kinksters were, if anything, healthier than a
vanilla control group. “The BDSM group scored higher than the control group on
Extraversion, Openness to Experience, and Conscientiousness, and lower on
Neuroticism and Agreeableness,” the authors reported. The BDSM group was also
less sensitive to rejection (even submissives scored no higher on this factor
than the control group did), and all three BDSM subsets—dominants, submissives,
and “switches”—outscored controls on “subjective well-being,” though the
difference was significant only for dominants. On measures designed to assess
healthy attachment, all the significant differences “showed the same pattern;
if scores were different, then the control group had the lowest scores,
followed by the subs, the switches, and finally the doms with the highest
scores.” What stood out most in this study was the difference between dominants
and everybody else: They scored higher on subjective well-being and lower on
anxious attachment, rejection sensitivity, and need for approval.
All these studies should be taken with a grain of salt. They
drew their BDSM samples from a few clubs and online forums that didn’t really
match the control groups—the BDSM samples, for instance, were older, more male,
and more educated—and might not represent a cross-section of kinksters
generally. From the way the papers were written, it’s obvious that the
researchers sympathized with BDSM and interpreted their data accordingly. But
the Australian study, with its more trustworthy random population sample, backs
them up. “Engagement in BDSM was not associated with higher levels of
psychological distress (i.e., feeling sad, nervous, hopeless, etc.),” the
authors reported. “Indeed, among men who participated in BDSM, the levels were
significantly lower. Among women, they were apparently higher but this did not
reach statistical significance.” For women in the Australian survey, “engagement
in BDSM was significantly related to having been imprisoned within the past 15
years.” But that wasn’t true for men.
5.
Is it exploitative? In the Dutch sample, men were
primarily tops (48 percent classified themselves as dominant, 33 percent as
submissive) while women were primarily bottoms (76 percent submissive, eight
percent dominant). In the California sample, 61 percent of the men were
exclusively or mainly dominant (26 percent were exclusively or mainly
submissive), while 69 percent of the women were exclusively or mainly
submissive (30 percent were exclusively or mainly dominant). The smaller
Canadian sample showed no such difference, and the authors said they “could not
find evidence that sadomasochists are anti-feminist.” But the gender gap bears
watching. If it shows up in other studies, it will raise hard questions about
the subordination of women, either by culture or—more disconcerting—by nature.
What does all this research add up to? Here are a few
tentative ideas. First, BDSM isn’t a single practice or population. It’s an
amalgamation of different people and fetishes. The spankers are different from
the branders. Most people who like collars want nothing to do with choking. The
populations sampled in the existing studies were largely soft-core—the Canadian
sample, for instance, was recruited from sites such as alt.personal.spanking
and alt.sex.bondage—and this tilt, while probably representative of BDSM as a
whole, makes it difficult to discern whether the heavy stuff is mentally
healthy or physically safe.
Second, the soft-core majority of kinksters might be defined
less by fetish than by sheer appetite. In the Australian study, for instance,
“Engagement in BDSM correlated strongly with a large number of sexual practice
measures associated with greater sexual activity and interest in sex.” The
authors of the Canadian paper agree: “Sadomasochists in this research reported
having had more sex partners and a greater likelihood of having explored
non-heterosexual experiences. They were also more likely to be sexually active
relative to non-sadomasochists. One might wonder, on the basis of these
findings, whether sadomasochists are simply individuals for whom sex and
sexuality play a relatively important role. One might wonder whether SM ought
to be understood best as a game explored by the sexually sophisticated and
adventurous …”
Third, what makes BDSM safe and mentally healthy for most of
its players is a combination of self-awareness, communication, and rules.
Subjective well-being, as the Dutch paper points out, is known to benefit from
recognition of “one’s own sexual identity and desires” as well as the ability
“to adequately and explicitly communicate these to sexual partners. As BDSM
play requires the explicit consent of the players regarding the type of actions
to be performed, their duration and intensity, and therefore involves careful
scrutiny and communication of one’s own sexual desires and needs, this may be
one possible explanation for the positive association between BDSM
practitioning and subjective well-being.” Community helps, too: According to
the Finnish paper, “social well-being appears to be associated with levels of
integration in sadomasochistic subcultures.”
In short, BDSM is a lot like other practices and
communities. It works when it’s well-governed and when its constituents are
well-formed. When they aren’t, and when the rules are unclear, the dangerous
ingredients at the core of this culture—domination, exploitation, violence—can
inflict serious harm. In the Finnish study, for instance, the authors reported
that kinksters who had previously been abused “visited a physician more often
because of injuries inflicted in sm-sex. This may suggest that they had difficulties
in setting appropriate limits.” The researchers concluded that “a small
subgroup of sm-practitioners seem to be both psychologically and socially
maladjusted.”
For these people, BDSM is a pathology. But for most of its
practitioners, it’s just a game. Our job is to keep it that way, by encouraging
the community—and allowing it—to police itself.
WILLIAM SALETAN
Will Saletan writes about politics, science, technology, and other stuff
for Slate. He’s the author
of Bearing Right.
Follow him on Twitter.
2 comments:
I find it amazing how far superior thewomen is ovwr man after reading this i cannot get on myknews fast enoufh i owe aervitude dearly for taking so long to give myself up tomy eternal servitude andpunishmwnt ihave much to earn but feel the grasp of my misstres i lpve the physcologica raimimg insted it made me put myself oit publicallysomething i would never do but i did it because i will ñow do anything asked i dont have to wory because im nobody and learned to beg and gibe up y oride for your oleasure i stand at the rhwshold of a kufe i ince jnew u nwed to be braunwashed and reowatedly i am read o should not no anything at allmy mouth is for your pussy and ass not talkung my dick is your toymy body us your prilerty i shall have no time to myself even when u slwep put headphonws ob telling me umnust a nake whire its tume women abuse and ise men make ñe a whore
UMMMMM.... I need to get some glasses - I think
Post a Comment